NSF Grant Funds Study of Plea Decisions
![Asst. Prof. of Psychology Miko Wilford speaks at a panel on problems with the plea system](/Images/Wilford%20panel%20photo-cropped_tcm18-305826.jpg?w=l)
02/20/2019
By Katharine Webster
There鈥檚 a heavy hand on the scales of justice 鈥 one that causes innocent defendants to plead guilty in large numbers, according to Asst. Prof. Miko Wilford.
That heavy hand is the power of prosecutors to offer dramatic 鈥渟entencing discounts鈥 in return for a guilty plea, Wilford says.
Before the 1980s, about 20 percent of criminal cases went to trial, but now fewer than 5 percent do. The change was fueled in part by the passage of mandatory minimum sentence and 鈥渢hree strikes鈥 laws. Now, the pressure is greater than ever for defendants to plead guilty rather than risk a conviction at trial, even when they are innocent, she says.
鈥淲hen a defendant is facing a 55-year mandatory minimum sentence and the prosecutor offers him a plea deal for 15 years, he is essentially being punished for exercising his constitutional right to a trial,鈥 she says.
Wilford, a psychology professor, was just awarded a five-year, $498,000 National Science Foundation CAREER grant to study how people make plea decisions. She hopes to restore some balance to the scales, doing research to inform policy changes that will reduce the number of false guilty pleas, especially by juveniles who don鈥檛 fully comprehend the consequences.
鈥淛uveniles are more prone to falsely plead guilty,鈥 she says. 鈥淒efense attorneys spend less time with juvenile clients because juveniles are less likely to assert themselves. Juveniles are more susceptible to pressure from prosecutors and other adults. And, even though there are protections in place for juveniles, at the end of the day, it鈥檚 their decision whether or not to plead guilty, even if they鈥檙e lucky enough to have both parents involved and their parents encourage them not to.鈥
The NSF grant builds on Wilford鈥檚 previous research, including her collaboration with Asst. Prof. Misha Rabinovich in the Art and Design Department and several students to create an online, animated plea-deal simulation.
In the demo version at PleaJustice.org, research participants 鈥減lay鈥 as a female driver who is charged with hitting a parked car and then leaving the scene of the accident. Some participants then see a flashback scene in which they 鈥渞emember鈥 hitting the car, while others see a flashback that indicates they are innocent.
![PleaJustice.org simulation for plea bargaining research](/Images/Pleajustice%20image-sized_tcm18-305825.jpg?w=l)
Then they鈥檙e offered a deal by the prosecutor: probation and a fine for a guilty plea, compared to jail time and higher costs if they reject the plea offer and they鈥檙e found guilty at trial. After choosing one or the other, they鈥檙e asked to explain the reasons for their choice.
Wilford鈥檚 pilot study yielded results similar to live, in-person experiments: Deep sentencing discounts, coupled with the prosecutor鈥檚 claim to have evidence showing the suspect at the scene, lead a significant number of 鈥渋nnocent鈥 people to plead guilty.
The more advanced version of the simulation that Wilford and Rabinovich are now developing includes a second crime scenario involving a shoplifting charge. Participants will start the simulation by creating an avatar who resembles them, which previous research shows enhances participants鈥 engagement. Also, they will get advice from a defense attorney before making a decision.
Wilford will be testing variables that change the likelihood of a guilty plea, including the defendant鈥檚 age and the difference between the plea offer and the penalty for a conviction at trial. She will administer the simulation either online or in-person to three different groups: college students, adults who are recruited online and adolescents at the .
Using the juveniles鈥 feedback, she also will develop an educational version of the software that defense attorneys can use to help juveniles understand their rights and how to assert them 鈥 as well as the consequences of pleading guilty or choosing not to under different scenarios.
鈥淛uveniles really need to be examined with their own microscope,鈥 she says. 鈥淒evelopmentally, there鈥檚 a disconnect between juveniles saying they understand that they have a curfew, for example, and connecting that to concrete consequences, like not being able to go to their junior prom.鈥
Wilford is joined on the grant by Asst. Prof. of PsychologyJoseph Gonzales, for his statistical expertise, Education Prof. Jill Lohmeier, who will help with educational evaluation, and Kelly Sutherland, a first-year Ph.D. student in applied psychology and prevention science.